Running head : Letter and word priming Orthographic , phonological , and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming Jeffrey

نویسندگان

  • Jeffrey S. Bowers
  • Gabriella Vigliocco
  • Richard Haan
چکیده

A series of experiments assessed masked priming for letters and words that are visually similar (SIM) and dissimilar (DIS) in upperand lower-case formats. For letters, robust DIS priming was obtained in a naming task, but this priming did not extend to a variety of non-naming tasks. For words, robust DIS priming was obtained in both naming and nonnaming tasks. SIM letter and word priming extended to all tasks, but the effects were generally small for letters. Based on the restricted set of conditions for DIS letter priming, we suggest that this priming is mediated by phonological-articulatory processes, and based on the generality of DIS word priming, we argue that abstract orthographic codes mediate these effects. Consistent with this conclusion, we report priming between homophones (for both letters and words) in a naming task, but little word homophone priming in a lexical decision task. Letter and word priming 3 Orthographic, phonological, and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming According to some theories of reading, words are recognized "holistically" by matching their overall shape to orthographic representations of complete words. On this view, readers can bypass letter identification and recognize words as complete visual patterns (e.g., Allen, Wallace, & Weber, 1995). According to most accounts, however, words are recognized via their constituent letters, and the overall shape and trans-letter features play little or no role in word identification (e.g., Adams, 1979; Besner & Johnston, 1989; Humphreys, Evett, & Quinlan, 1990; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981). From this perspective, an understanding of the mental representations and processes involved in letter identification is an essential first step for any theory of reading. Despite the critical role that letter identification plays in most theories of reading, relatively little work has directly assessed the representations and processes involved in letter identification. Instead, researchers typically investigate word (or pseudoword) identification, and the results are assumed to have implications for letters (e.g., Humphreys, Evett, & Taylor, 1982; McClelland, 1976). Of course, the validity of this inference is only warranted on the assumption that letters and words are equivalent on the dimensions under investigation -an assumption that is rarely assessed. If it turns out that letters and words are represented and/or processed in different ways, then the results obtained with words are uninformative with regards to theories of letter identification. In the present paper, we directly test the letter/word equivalence assumption by comparing masked priming for letters and words using a number of related tasks. Contrary to the common view, we report evidence that qualitatively different mechanisms Letter and word priming 4 underlie letter and word priming; namely, phonlogical-articulatory processes and orthographic representations, respectively. The masked priming paradigm we employ in the present series of experiments includes a number of related tasks that assess the influence of an initial letter or letter string (prime) on the processing of a second letter or letter string (target) when the prime (and in some cases the target) is difficult to identify due to its brief display and the inclusion of preand/or post-masks. For example, in the task introduced by Forster & Davis (1984), a pattern mask (e.g., #####) is replaced by a prime displayed in lower case letters for 60 ms, which in turn is replaced by a target displayed in upper case letters for 500 ms. Participants are required to make lexical decisions to the targets, and priming is measured as the difference in reaction time for related (e.g., table/TABLE) compared to unrelated (e.g., flute/TABLE) prime/target pairs. In another task introduced by Evett and Humphreys (1981), the prime and target are both briefly flashed and masked, and participants are asked to identify the target. Priming, in this latter case, is measured as the difference between the proportions of target identifications when the prime is related compared to when it is unrelated to the target.1 Researchers employing the priming paradigm often adopt the letter/word equivalence assumption described above. For example, the finding that cross-case word priming is equivalent for prime/target pairs that are visually similar (e.g., kiss/KISS) or dissimilar (e.g., read/READ) in their upperand lower-case formats has led to the view that words and letters are coded in an abstract format within the orthographic system (Humphreys, et al., 1982; Humphreys, Evett, & Quinlan, 1990). However, adopting this same implicit assumption, Lukatela and Turvey (1994) recently questioned this conclusion Letter and word priming 5 based on their observation of a co-occurrence of cross-case and homophone priming. That is, whenever they observed priming between upperand lower-case words (e.g., prime = SAIL, target = sail), they also obtained priming between homophones (e.g., prime = SALE, target = sail), suggesting that this cross-case and homophone priming are mediated by the same representations; namely, phonological codes. Critical for present purposes, they extended this argument and concluded that all reports of cross-case word priming are mediated by phonological codes, and then questioned the need to infer abstract orthographic word and letter codes. Note that in both cases, the authors used priming results obtained with words in order to make claims regarding letter representations: Humphreys et al. claim that the priming results support the existence of abstract letter codes, Lukatela and Turvey claim the opposite. Although most priming studies have included words as target materials, there are a few experiments that have directly assessed priming for single letters (Arguin & Bub, 1995; Jacobs & Grainger, 1991; Jacobs, Grainger & Ferrand, 1995). Since these studies serve as the starting point for the present investigation, we describe them below in some detail. Jacobs and Grainger (1991) asked participants to classify target characters as letters or non-letters as quickly as possible. On each experimental trial, an initial mask (#) was presented for 500 ms, which was replaced by a prime (letter or non-letter) displayed between 20 and 80 ms, which was replaced by a mask (#) for 16 ms, which in turn was replaced by the target that remained on the screen until participants responded (e.g., #a#A). The critical finding for our purposes was that participants responded faster when letters were repeated in lower/upper case (e.g., a/A) compared to when different letters Letter and word priming 6 were presented (e.g., b/A) -as is the case for words in the lexical decision task. They concluded that abstract orthographic letter codes mediated the priming. This experiment, however, was recently criticized by Arguin and Bub (1995) who noted that the letters employed by Jacobs and Grainger (1991) were not selected in terms of their visual similarity in upperand lower-case format in the critical experiment. Some prime/target pairs were visually similar (e.g., prime = c; target = C), while others were dissimilar (e.g., prime = d; target = D). Thus, it is possible that the cross-case priming was restricted to the visually similar prime/target pairs, in which case the results do not provide evidence for the existence of abstract letter codes.2 Indeed, when Arguin and Bub (1995) repeated this experiment using a set of visually dissimilar letters in upper/lower case, they failed to obtain cross-case priming. Similarly, Jacobs et al. (1995) failed to obtain crosscase letter priming using a different baseline condition (i.e., prime intensity) even though they used the same set of letters as Jacobs and Grainger (1991). Although Arguin and Bub (1995) failed to obtain cross-case priming for visually dissimilar letters using an alphabetic decision task, they did obtain robust priming for these same items when participants were required to name the targets. In order to rule out the possibility that phonological representations mediated priming in this condition, the authors assessed priming between phonologically similar letters (e.g., prime = p, target =b). They reasoned that if phonological representations mediated letter priming in the naming task, then some priming should extend to phonologically similar prime/target pairs. Since they did not find priming in this latter condition, they concluded that the cross-case priming was mediated by abstract orthographic codes. Letter and word priming 7 In order to account for the contrasting priming results in the alphabetic decision and naming tasks, the authors focused on the different requirements of the two tasks. Consider first the alphabetic decision task. They argued that participants performed this task by monitoring the overall level of letter activation within the orthographic system, and responded "letter" when the activation passed some threshold, and responded "non-letter" otherwise. That is, performance on the alphabetic decision task was made in response to some global assessment of letter activation rather than in response to the identification of a specific letter target. Furthermore, they speculated that participants completed the alphabetic decision task by monitoring the global activation of so-called letter tokens, which were defined as visually specific representations of letters that are initially activated during reading, and that map onto abstract letter types (e.g., "a" and "A" are different letter tokens, that map onto the abstract letter type "A"). Since performance on the alphabetic decision task is presumed to be mediated by letter tokens (that do not represent the equivalence of upperand lower case letters), the absence of cross-case priming in this task is readily explained. By contrast, in the naming task, the overall activation of letter tokens is uninformative with regards to the particular letter name that must be produced. Accurate performance here requires participants to identify the particular letter codes that map onto phonological representations -and these are presumed to be the abstract letter types. Since abstract letter types are relevant to completing the naming task, cross-case letter priming is obtained. To summarize, Arguin and Bub (1995) argue that letter priming is mediated by abstract letter types when participants are required to specifically identify target letters, and by visually specific letter tokens when general activation of letter tokens will suffice. Letter and word priming 8 Thus, two different types of orthographic codes are assumed to mediate withinand crosscase priming. The results reported by Arguin and Bub (1995) are important in a number of respects. First, these results provide some evidence compatible with the existence of abstract letter codes based on a direct assessment of letter priming (and not inferred from word priming results). Second, the contrasting results obtained with the alphabetic decision and naming tasks highlight the need to include multiple priming tasks before making any conclusions regarding the representations involved in letter identification (see Forster & Davis, 1991; Davis & Forster, 1994 for similar conclusion regarding word and pseudoword priming). Third, the results demonstrate the importance of selecting letters that are visually dissimilar in their upperand lower-case formats when assessing crosscase priming (see also Bowers, 1996; Bowers, Arguin & Bub, 1996). There are reasons, however, to question Arguin and Bub's (1995) account of letter priming. In particular, we believe that a phonological account of letter priming has not been adequately ruled out. As noted above, Arguin and Bub (1995) rejected a phonological explanation based on their failure to obtain priming between phonologically similar letters. It is important to note, however, that phonological priming may have a narrow generalization gradient, in which case priming would be expected between upperlower case letters (that share the same name), but not between letters with similar names. Indeed, this appears to be the case for words, where robust priming is obtained between homophones in a naming task (e.g., Luketela & Turvey, 1994), but in the same task, no priming is obtained between words that rhyme (Forster & Davis, 1991). Furthermore, it is possible that an articulatory onset effect is responsible for DIS letter priming in the naming Letter and word priming 9 task. That is, participants may prepare to articulate the prime prior to the encoding of the target, which in turns facilitates target naming when prime and target share the initial phoneme, and inhibits target naming otherwise (see Forster & Davis, 1991). We report here a series of six experiments which attempt to shed further light into the representations and processes that support both letter and word priming. In contrast to the view of Arguin and Bub (1995), we provide evidence that phonological-articulatory codes do indeed mediate letter priming. And in contrast to Luketela and Turvey (1994) and others, we show that orthographic codes support word priming. Experiment 1 The goals of this experiment were two-fold. First, we assessed cross-case priming for letters that are visually similar and dissimilar in their upperand lower-case formats in order to test the claim that cross-case letter priming is restricted to visually similar items in the alphabetic decision task (Arguin & Bub, 1995). Second, we assessed cross-case priming for words that are visually similar and dissimilar in upperand lower-case using a lexical decision task in order to determine whether word and letter priming is equally sensitive to a case manipulation. Method Participants. Twenty-four participants completed the letter priming task and 24 participants completed the word priming task. Participants were undergraduate students from University of Arizona and Rice University. Materials and Design: Letters. The target letters consisted in eight letters that are visually dissimilar (DIS) in upperand lower-case, and eight letters that are visually similar (SIM) in these formats, as determined by a visually similarity matrix reported by Boles and Letter and word priming 10 Clifford (1989). The DIS letters were: A/a, B/b, D/d, E/e, G/g, L/l, Q/q, R/r and the SIM letters were: C/c, K/k, I/i, O/o, P/p, S/s, U/u, and W/w. An additional eight nonalphabetic characters were selected to serve as non-letter targets in the alphabetic decision task ($, +, &, ?, %, =, ^, and @). The letter experiment included Prime/Target Relation (primes and targets were repeated vs. primes and targets were non-repeated) and Item Type (SIM vs. DIS ) as within-subject factors. Each letter was presented in the repeated and non-repeated conditions three times, leading to 24 trials per condition, for a total of 96 letter responses. A corresponding number of non-letter trails were included. In the non-repeated condition, SIM letters were randomly paired with the other SIM letters and DIS letters were randomly paired with the other DIS letters. Words. For the word priming experiment, eight DIS and eight SIM words were selected so that they were composed of the corresponding DIS and SIM letters. The DIS words were: able, bled, bald, deal, area, edge, gale, and read; and the SIM words were: cook, chop, kiss, soon, stop, pick, upon, and pump. An additional set of eight SIM and eight DIS random letter strings (e.g., ebqd, kpso) were created for the nonword trials in the lexical decision task. The inclusion of random letter strings as the non-word targets insured that non-words were easily distinguished from words, which we considered important given that letters were easily distinguished from non-letters in the alphabetic decision task. The word experiment included Prime/Target Relation (repeated vs. non-repeated) and Item Type (SIM vs. DIS ) as within-subject factors. Each word was presented in the repeated and non-repeated conditions three times, leading to 24 trials per condition, for a Letter and word priming 11 total of 96 word responses. In the non-repeated condition, SIM words were randomly paired with the other SIM words and DIS words were randomly paired with the other DIS words. A corresponding number of nonword trails were included. Procedure. In the letter experiment, each trial was organized in the following way. First a pattern mask (#) was presented for 500 ms which was replaced by the prime (e.g., a) presented for 50 ms, immediately followed by the target (e.g., A), which was displayed for 500 ms. In the word experiment, each trial was organized in the same way except that the mask consisted of four pound signs (####). For both experiments, participants completed a set of 18 practice trials prior to the experimental trials. The same set of items were included in the practice and experimental trials, and participants were not informed that the initial trials were practice. In this and all subsequent experiments, stimuli were presented on a computer-controlled video display using the DMASTER system developed by K.I. Forster and J.C. Forster at the University of Arizona. This system synchronizes the display with the video raster so that precise control of stimulus onsets and offsets is possible. Participants in the letter and word priming experiments were instructed to press the right shift key as quickly as possible when an upper-case letter or word was displayed, and to press the left shift-key whenever a non-letter or non-word was displayed. Participants received feed-back on each trial. Results In this experiment, as in subsequent experiments that measured response latencies, the effects of outliers were curtailed by cutoffs established 2 standard deviations above and below the mean of each participant. Data from trials on which an error occurred were Letter and word priming 12 discarded. Separate analyses of the subject (F1) and the item (F2) means were carried out. Letters. The mean alphabetic decision latencies and error rates in the various conditions are shown in Table 1a. An overall Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) carried out on the RT data failed to reveal a main effect of priming [F1(1,23) = 1.29, MSe = 819.53, p >.25; F2(1,46) = 2.61, MSe = 526.82, p = .11], nor an interaction between priming and letter-type [F1(1,23) = 1.6, MSe = 686.13, p >.2; F2(1,46) = 3.09, MSe = 526.82, p = .09], suggesting a failure to obtain priming for both the DIS (0 ms) and SIM (13 ms) letters. However, the SIM priming approached significance [F1(1,23) =2.6, MSe = 829.61, p =.12; F2(1,23) = 5.36, MSe = 558.09, p < .05]. An overall ANOVA carried out on the error scores revealed a main effect of letter type, reflecting a slightly higher error rate for SIM (6.2%) compared to DIS (4.4%) letters [F1(1,23) = 4.64, MSe = 17.11, p < .05; F2(1,46) = 4.47, MSe = 17.83, p < .05]. No other effect approached significance. Words. The mean lexical decision latencies and error rates in the various conditions are shown in Table 1b. An analysis carried out on the response latencies showed a significant effect of priming [F1(1,23) = 41.66, MSe = 215.93, p < .001; F2(1,46) = 27.76. MSe = 473.16, p < .001], and no interaction between priming and word-type [F1 < 1; F2 < 1], indicating that the same amount of priming was obtained for DIS (25 ms) and SIM (22 ms) words. An analysis on the error scores failed to reveal any significant effects [F1 values < 3.35, p values > .05; F2 values < 1.77, p values > .15]. It should be noted that the average latency for participants to make a "word" response (507 ms, collapsing across conditions) is similar to the average latency to make a "letter" Letter and word priming 13 response (494 ms, collapsing across conditions). Thus, the contrasting letter and word results do not appear to be related to differences in task difficulty. It worth pointing out that the inclusion of random letter strings as distactors in the lexical decision task may have reduced the overall size of word priming, given that the 24 ms priming effect (collapsing across SIM and DIS words) is much smaller than the typical effect of 50-60 ms (e.g. Forster & Davis, 1984). Forster (1992) also found that including random letter string distractors, as opposed to pseudoword distractors (e.g., blap), reduces priming. Discussion The main results of this experiment can be summarized as follows. For letters, we failed to obtain DIS priming in the alphabetic decision task, consistent with Arguin and Bub (1995). We also observed a trend for SIM letter priming, suggesting that the crosscase priming reported by Jacobs and Grainger (1991) may have been due to the inclusion of SIM letters in their experiment. For words, we did obtain robust DIS and SIM priming. This DIS word priming is hardly surprising given the many studies that have found cross-case priming for words under similar conditions (e.g., Forster & Davis, 1984). Nevertheless, the fact that robust priming was obtained for DIS words under experimental conditions that failed to support DIS letter priming makes the null letter results more striking. Thus, it appears that there are interesting differences in the representations or processes responsible for letter and word priming. Experiment 2 In this experiment, we attempted to replicate the robust DIS letter priming reported by Arguin and Bub (1995) when the naming task was employed. Method Letter and word priming 14 Participants. Twenty-four students from the University of Arizona completed the experiment in return for course credit. Materials and Design. The same set of letters used in Experiment 1 were included here. The design was the same except that all non-letter trials were dropped. Accordingly, there were 96 trials, with each letter repeated three times in the repeated and non-repeated conditions. Procedure. Each experimental trial consisted of a mask (#) presented for 500 ms, replaced by a lower-case letter displayed for 50 ms (the prime), replaced in turn by an upper-case letter (the target). Target letters remained on the screen for 500 ms or until participants responded. Participants' task was to name upper-case letters as quickly as possible. The experimenter monitored participants' responses, and discarded any trials in which a naming error or a dysfluency occurred. Results The mean naming latencies and error rates in each condition are shown in Table 2. The overall analysis of the naming latencies showed a significant priming effect [F1(1,23 = 33.57, MSe = 303.11, p < .001; F2(1,46) = 63.58, MSe = 328.51, p < .001], and a significant interaction between priming and letter-type[F1(1,23) = 5.55, MSe = 303.11, p < .05; F2(1,46) = 4.82, MSe = 328.51, p < .05], indicating that a larger amount of priming was obtained for SIM (38 ms) compared to DIS (21 ms) letters. However, the priming for DIS letters was also significant [F1(1,23) = 22.34, MSe = 242.53, p < .001; F2(1,23) = 18.33, MSe = 299.08, p < .001]. It is worth noting that in Experiment 1 we also found more priming for SIM (13 ms) compared to DIS (0 ms) letters. It is possible that the larger amount of SIM priming in the alphabetic decision and naming tasks can be Letter and word priming 15 explained in terms of the contribution of perceptual processes common to both tasks that take advantage of the visual similarity of the prime and targets. For example, the advantage of SIM over DIS letters might reflect the contribution of letter tokens that specify the perceptual details of letters, consistent with Arguin & Bub (1995). The basis of the SIM-DIS difference in priming is further considered in the General Discussion. An analysis of the error data revealed a main effect of priming [F1(1,23) = 4.93, MSe = 5.32, p < .05; F2( 1,46) = 7.29, MSe = 3.6, p < .01] and an interaction of priming with letter type [F1(1,23) = 6.94, MSe = 2.62, p < .05; F2(1,46) = 5.06, MSe =3.60, p < .01], reflecting a greater percentage of errors in the non-repeated (1.5%) compared to the repeated (.45%) condition, particularly for the SIM letters Discussion The critical finding of the present experiment is that we obtained priming for DIS letters when the naming task was employed, consistent with Arguin and Bub (1995) . This finding, in combination with the results of Experiment 1, raises the same question that Arguin and Bub confronted; namely, why is DIS letter priming obtained in some tasks and not in others? According to Arguin and Bub, the naming task requires participants to make an absolute identification for each letter type prior to naming it, whereas the alphabetic decision task only requires participants to monitor the global level of activation of letter tokens. That is, the contrasting priming results reflect the different orthographic codes used in these tasks. As noted above, however, it is possible that phonological codes or articulatory overlap supported the DIS letter priming when the naming task was employed (see Forster & Davis, 1991; Lukatela & Turvey, 1994, for compatible evidence using word targets). Letter and word priming 16 Note, the view that letter priming reflects phonological-articulatory processes can readily account for the lack of DIS priming in the alphabetic decision task, since this task does not require participants to phonologically encode or articulate letters. The next two experiments are designed to test the relative merits of these two explanations. In these experiments we required participants to specifically identify letter and word targets without stressing the naming response. According to Arguin and Bub's account, DIS letter priming should be obtained in these conditions, whereas according to a phonological-articulatory explanation, no priming should be found. Experiment 3 In the present experiment, participants categorized letter targets as vowels or consonants as quickly as possible. This task requires the identification of particular letters, but does not require, at least in principle, access to phonological representations. A corresponding word task was constructed in which participants categorized target words as nouns or verbs as quickly as possible. Again, the absolute identification of the target words is necessary, but phonological encoding is not. Method. Participants. Twenty-four students from the University of Arizona completed the letter priming task, and an additional 24 students from Rice University completed the word priming task. Materials and Design. The target letters were composed of 4 DIS letters, 2 of which were vowels (a, and e) and 2 of which were consonants (d, and g), as well as 4 SIM letters, 2 of which were vowels (o and u) and 2 of which were consonants (s, and k). Corresponding to these letters, 4 DIS words were selected, 2 of which were verbs (earn Letter and word priming 17 and tend) and 2 of which were nouns (gate and tree), as well as 4 SIM words, 2 of which were verbs (swim and push) and 2 of which were nouns (sock and wool). The letter and word experiments included two within-subjects factors: (a) Prime/Target Relation (repeated vs. non-repeated) and (b) Item Type (SIM vs. DIS). Each letter and word was presented in the repeated and non-repeated conditions six times, leading to 24 trials per condition, for a total of 96 word and 96 letter trials. For both letters and words, primes and targets in the non-repeated condition were randomly paired with other items from the same consonant/vowel or noun/verb set. That is, vowels were randomly paired with other vowels (regardless of the DIS vs. SIM status), and nouns were randomly paired with nouns (regardless of DIS vs. SIM status). RTs were taken from all responses. Procedure. As in the previous experiments, the display included a mask of one or four characters (# or ####) presented for 500 ms, followed by the letter or word prime presented in lower-case for 50 ms, which in turn was replaced by the target item in uppercase for 500 ms (e.g., #-a-A, or ####-earn-EARN). Participants in the letter categorization experiment were required to press the right shift key as quickly as possible when the target was a vowel, and the left shift-key when the target was a consonant. Similarly, participants in the word priming experiment pressed the right shift key as quickly as possible when the target was a verb, and the left-shift key when the target was a noun. Eighteen practice trials preceded the experimental trials in both experiments. Results. Letter and word priming 18 Letters The categorization latencies and error rates in each condition are shown in Table 3a. No main effect of priming for RTs was obtained [F1(1,23) = 2.33, MSe = 318.85, p = .14; F2(1,46) = 1.46, MSe = 755.32, p = .23] and the interaction between priming and letter-type did not approach significance [F1 < 1 and F2 <1]. Nevertheless, a simple contrast revealed a slight trend for SIM letter priming (9 ms) [F1(1,23) = 1.98, MSe = 508.90, p = .17; F2(1,23) = 1.85, MSe = 465.13, p = .19]. The ANOVA carried out on the error data revealed a main effect of letter-type, [F1(1,23) = 4.88, MSe = 18.03, p < .05; F2(1,46) = 5.27, MSe = 16.54, p < .05], reflecting the higher proportion of errors with DIS (4.7%) compared to SIM (2.8%) letters. No other effect achieved significance. Words. The categorization latencies and error rates in each condition are shown in Table 3b. An overall ANOVA carried out on the RTs revealed a main effect of priming [F1(1,23) = 17.48, MSe = 2068.84, p < .001; F2(1,46) = 29.79, MSe = 1153.22, p < .001], and no interaction between priming and word-type [F1 and F2 < 1], reflecting the equivalent priming for DIS (34 ms) and SIM (34 ms) words. The analysis of the errors did not reveal any significant effects [all F1 values < 2.28, all p values > .13; all F2 values < 1.83, all p values > .18]. Discussion. The critical finding in the present experiment is that no priming was obtained for DIS letters in a categorization task that required participants to make absolute identifications of target letters. This result contrasts with the robust letter priming in the naming task reported in Experiment 2. Thus, priming for DIS letters does not depend upon whether participants specifically identify the target letters, contrary to the conclusion of Arguin & Bub (1995). The SIM letter priming was non-significant, but there was a Letter and word priming 19 small trend for priming, consistent with the prior results. A second important finding is that robust word priming was obtained in a noun/verb categorization task, and this priming was equal in size for both DIS and SIM words. These results parallel the lexical decision results reported in Experiment 1. Experiment 4 In order to provide additional support for the claim that DIS letter-priming is restricted to tasks that emphasize phonological-articulatory processes, we assessed letter priming in a perceptual identification task. The challenge for participants is to identify specific target letters under degraded perceptual conditions, and thus we reasoned that orthographic processes are stressed to a greater extent in this task compared to the naming task in which items are presented in the clear. Thus, according to the phonological-articulatory account, DIS letter priming should be minimal. A parallel task with words was also conducted. If orthographic codes support word priming, DIS word priming should be robust. Method Participants. Twenty-four students from Rice University completed the letter identification task, and another group of 26 students from the same subject pool completed the word identification task. Materials and Design. Seven DIS letters (a, b, d, e, g, q, r) and seven SIM letters (c, k, o, s, u, w, z) were included in the letter experiment. In the word priming task, 56 DIS and 56 SIM words were included, all four letters in length, half of which were low in frequency (median frequency = 4, range = 1-17 occurrences per million), and half of which were high in frequency (median frequency = 123, range = 15 -1303 occurrences per Letter and word priming 20 million; Kucera & Francis, 1967). All DIS and SIM words were composed of at least three DIS or SIM letters, respectively. In both experiments there were two within-subjects factors. (a) Prime/Target Relation (repeated vs. non-repeated); and (b) Item Type (SIM vs. DIS ). In the letter experiment, each letter was presented four times in the repeated condition, and four times in the non-repeated condition, leading to 112 trials. SIM letters were randomly paired with other SIM letters, and DIS letters were randomly paired with other DIS letters in the non-repeated condition. In the word experiment, words were not repeated, preventing participants from guessing the target on the basis of preceding exposures to the same item.3 Two versions of the test were created so that each word was presented in the repeated and the non-repeated conditions, creating a fully counter-balanced design. Thus, each subject completed 112 experimental word trials. In the non-repeated condition, SIM words were randomly paired with other SIM words, and DIS words were randomly paired with other DIS words. Procedure. Each experimental trial consisted of a mask (# or ####) presented for 500 ms, replaced by a lower case letter or word prime, replaced in turn by the target letter or word in upper-case, finally replaced by another mask (# or ####) for 500 ms. In order to equate the performance as closely as possible for the two materials, prime and target letters were displayed for 34 ms each, while prime and target words were displayed for 17 ms each.4 Participants were asked to identify any letter or word that they saw, and they were encouraged to guess. Responses were considered correct if participants identified either the prime or the target, which provides a conservative test of priming, given that two possible responses are correct in the baseline condition, and only one response is Letter and word priming 21 correct in the repeated condition. We adopted this criterion because it is rather arbitrary to consider the identification of one item correct and the identification of the other item as incorrect under conditions in which the prime and target are presented for the same duration. Results Letters. The identification rate in each condition is shown in Table 4a. As can be seen in this table, robust letter priming was obtained for SIM letters (20% improvement) and relatively little priming was obtained for DIS letters (5% improvement). An overall ANOVA revealed a significant effect of priming [F1(1, 25) = 49.30, MSe = .01, p < .001; F2(1,54) = 18.01, MSe = .03, p < .001] and an interaction between priming and lettertype [F1(1,25) = 11.92, MSe = .01, p < .01; F2(1,54) = 6.12, MSe = .03, p < .05], reflecting the greater amount of priming for SIM compared to DIS letters. A simple contrast failed to reveal priming for DIS letters, although the effect approached significance [F1(1,25) = 3.05, MSe = .03, p = .09; F2(1,27) = 1.87, MSe = .04, p = .19]. Words. The identification rate in each condition is shown in Table 4b. An ANOVA carried out on these data revealed a priming effect [F1(1,22) = 112.07, MSe = .06, p < .001; F2(1,52) = 291.99, MSe= .03, p < .001)], and the interaction between priming and word-type approached significance[F1(1,22) = 7.03, MSe .02, p < .05; F2(1,52) = 2,52, MSe = .04, p = .12], reflecting the greater amount of priming for SIM words (.43) compared to DIS words (.34). In addition, the interaction between priming and word frequency approached significance [F1(1,22) = 2.46, MSe = .20, p = .13; F2(1,52) = 3.27, MSe = .03, p = .08], reflecting a tendency to show more priming for high(.42) compared to low(.35) frequency words. Letter and word priming 22 Discussion. The critical finding of the present experiment is that robust priming was obtained for DIS words, whereas only a nonsignificant trend was obtained for DIS letters under similar test conditions. These results closely match the results of Experiment 3 in which a categorization task was employed, and thus further challenge the view according to which priming is obtained for DIS letters as long as participants identify specific letters. Instead, the results are consistent with the view that letter priming is restricted to tasks that emphasize phonological-articulatory processes -i.e., the naming task. Note, even the small trend for DIS letter priming in the identification task may be mediated by phonological codes, given past finding that phonological codes contribute to priming in the identification task in some circumstances (e.g., Humphreys, Evett & Taylor, 1982; Perfetti et al., 1988; Perfetti & Bell, 1991; see Verstaen, Humphreys, Olson, & D'Ydewalle, 1996 for evidence that the phonological priming is the product of strategic rather than automatic processes). Finally, it is worth noting that we found more priming for SIM compared to DIS words, in contrast to previous experiments in which we found a similar amount of priming. A related finding has been reported in the literature by Davis and Forster (1994), who found that word priming is only influenced by the perceptual overlap of the prime and target when both items are presented briefly. (The perceptual overlap had no effect when the targets were presented in free view.) Thus, it appears that visually specific perceptual codes play a larger role in priming when targets are presented briefly. Note, this may also explain why SIM letter priming was larger in the present identification task compared to the previous tasks in which letter targets were presented for 500 ms. Letter and word priming 23 Experiment 5 The previous experiments provide a clear-cut picture regarding the conditions in which DIS letter and word priming is obtained. That is, robust DIS letter priming is restricted to the naming task, whereas robust DIS word priming is obtained in all the tests we used; namely, lexical decision, naming, noun/verb categorization, and perceptual identification tasks. These contrasting findings are all the more striking, given that the letter and word tasks were closely matched. Thus, the present set of results suggest that qualitatively different representations or processes mediate priming for letters and words. One hypothesis that can readily account for this pattern of results is that phonological-articulatory processes mediate DIS letter priming, whereas abstract orthographic codes mediate DIS word priming. However, before this hypothesis can be strongly endorsed, it is important to provide independent evidence in support of this position. The final two experiments were carried-out in an attempt to provide this evidence. In Experiment 5, we assessed priming between word homophones in a naming and a lexical decision tasks. If word priming is largely supported by abstract orthographic codes in the lexical decision task and by phonological-articulatory codes in the naming task, then homophone priming should be more robust in the latter task. Participants. Twenty-for students from the University of Arizona completed the naming task and 24 students from Rice University completed the lexical decision task. Materials and Design. The same set of homophone pairs included in Appendix A of Lukatela and Turvey's (1994) study were included in the present experiment. One member of each pair served as prime, the other as target. This list was divided in two sets: In half of the pairs, the prime was higher in frequency than the target (mean frequency of Letter and word priming 24 prime and target are 173 and 12 occurrences per million, respectively; Kucera & Francis, 1967), and in half of the pairs, the target was higher in frequency than the prime (mean frequency of prime and target are 18 and 97 occurrence per million, respectively). The members of the homophone pairs that served as prime vs. target in the present experiment were slightly different than in the Lukatela and Turvey (1994) study because we required targets to be composed of either the same number or more letters than the prime. This insured that targets served as adequate masks for primes. In the baseline condition, we included the same set of control words as Lukatela and Turvey, that were matched with the targets so that (a) they had no letters in common (or in rare cases, just one letter, but in a different position), (b) they had the same number of letters, (c) they were approximately the same frequency, and (d) they were not prominent associates of their corresponding target. An additional 96 pronounceable nonword targets were constructed that were matched with word targets in terms of number of letters. All the nonwords were one letter different from real words, and no pseudohomophones were included. Nonword targets were presented in upper-case letters and were only used in the lexical decision experiment. The experiment included three conditions: A condition in which the prime and target were repeated (e.g., prime = sail, target = SAIL), a condition in which the prime and target were homophones (e.g., prime = sale, target = SAIL), and a condition in which the prime was unrelated to target (e.g., prime = butt, target = SAIL). Three lists were constructed so that each homophone target was presented in the three conditions equally often, producing a counter-balanced design. Consistent with the earlier studies, primes were presented in lower-case letters, and targets in upper-case. This contrasts with the Letter and word priming 25 Lukatela and Turvey (1994) study, where the primes and targets were presented in upperand lower-case, respectively. Procedure. Experimental trials in both the naming and lexical decision task consisted of a mask composed of eight pound signs (########) presented for 500 ms, replaced by the lower-case prime displayed for 50 ms, replaced in turn by the target word in upper-case letters. Target words remained on the screen for 500 ms or until participants responded. Participants in the naming task were asked to name the target word as quickly as possible. The experimenter monitored participants responses, and discarded any trials in which a naming error or a dysfluency occurred. Participants in the lexical decision task were required to press the right shift key as quickly as possible when words were presented, and the left shift-key when non-words were presented. Results. Naming. The mean naming latencies and error rates in each condition are shown in Table 5a. As can be seen in this table, the priming effects for RTs generally conform to the findings reported by Lukatela and Turvey (1994): Robust repetition (40 ms) and homophone (28 ms) priming was obtained. An overall ANOVA carried out on the RT data revealed a main effect of priming [F1(2,42) = 22.73, MSe = 860.50, p < .001; F2(2, 180) = 49.71, MSe = 836.33, p < .001], and no interaction between the relative frequency of the prime/targets and the effect of priming. [F1(2,42) = 1.08, MSe = 695.99, p = .35; F2(2, 180) = 1.86, MSe = 836.33, p = .16). An analysis of the error scores also revealed a main effect of priming [F1(2,42) = 11.32, MSe = 14.03, p < .01; F2( 2,180) = 8.73, MSe = 36.53, p < .01], reflecting the greater percentage of errors in the non-repeated (5.1%) compared to the repeated (2.21%) and homophone (1.69%) conditions. In addition, the Letter and word priming 26 interaction between priming and relative frequency of prime-targets was significant [F1(2,42) = 3.55, MSe= 18.56, p < .05; F2(2,180) = 3.62, MSe = 36.53, p < .05], reflecting the high percentage of errors in the high-low condition for non-repeated words (6.8%). In order to distinguish between repetition and homophone priming, two separate ANOVAs were carried out on the RT data. The ANOVA assessing repetition priming revealed a main effect of priming [F1(1,21) = 33.67, MSe = 1105.21, p < .001; F2(1, 90) = 75.35, MSe = 1051.88, p < .001], and the interaction between repetition priming and the relative frequency of items also approached significance [F1(1,21) = 2.88, MSe = 430.19, p = .11; F2(1,90) = 2.35, MSe = 1051.88, p = .13], reflecting a trend for more priming in the high/low (46 ms) compared to low/high (33 ms) frequency conditions. The ANOVA assessing homophone priming also showed an overall priming effect [F1(1,21) = 27.39, MSe = 657.55, p < .001; F2(1, 90) = 46.76, MSe = 811.36, p < .001], and the interaction between homophone priming and relative frequency of items approached significance [F1(1,21) = 1.25, MSe = 295.53, p = .28; F2( 1,90) = 2.69, MSe = 811.36, p = .11], reflecting a trend for more homophone priming in the low/high (33 ms) compared to high/low (21 ms) frequency conditions. Lexical Decision. The mean lexical decision latencies and error rates in each condition are shown in Table 5b. In sharp contrast with the results obtained in the naming task, priming was largely restricted to the repetition condition (39 ms), with little priming for homophones (6 ms). An overall ANOVA carried out on the RT data revealed a significant amount of priming [F1(2,42) = 30.71, MSe = 686.84, p < .001; F2(2, 180) = 13.26, MSe = 3019.52, p < .001], and the interaction between priming and the relative Letter and word priming 27 frequency of the prime/targets approached significance [F1(2,42) = 2.33, MSe = 624.80, p = .11; F2(2, 180) = 1.44, MSe = 4278.82, p = .24]. An analysis on the error scores revealed a main effect of priming [F1(2,42) = 6.66, MSe = 42.11, p < .01; F2(2,180) = 5.77, MSe = 97.11, p < .01], reflecting the smaller percentage of errors in the repeated (7.3%) compared to the homophone (10.0%) and non-repeated (12.1%) conditions, as well as an effect of the relative frequency of prime/targets [F1(1,21) = 45.97, MSe = 52.05, p < .01; F2(1, 90) = 12.26, MSe = 391.06, p < .01]. In order to distinguish repetition from homophone priming, two separate ANOVAs were carried out on the RT data. The ANOVA that assessed repetition priming revealed a main effect of priming [F1(1,21) = 41.31, MSe = 887.44, p < .001; F2(1, 90) = 23.76, MSe = 2747.52, p < .001], and no interaction between repetition priming and the relative frequency [F1(1,21) = 2.80, MSe = 326.30, p = .11; F2( 1,90) < 1], reflecting the similar amount of priming in the high/low (46 ms) and low/high (33 ms) frequency conditions. By contrast, the ANOVA assessing homophone priming failed to show an overall priming effect [F1(1,21) = 1.69, MSe = 582.56, p = .21; F2(1, 90) < 1], although the interaction between homophone priming and the relative frequency of items did approach significance [F1(1,21) = 3.61, MSe = 802.15, p = .07; F2( 1,90) = 2.74, MSe = 3156.94, p = .10], suggesting that homophone priming was in fact obtained in the high/low (17 ms) but not in the low/high (-4 ms) frequency conditions. Discussion Two main results were obtained in the present experiment. First, and consistent with the findings of Lukatela and Turvey (1994), we obtained robust homophone priming in the naming task. Based on these and related findings (e.g., Forster & Davis, 1991), it Letter and word priming 28 seems clear that phonological-articulatory processes do indeed support priming for words when the naming task is employed. Second, when the lexical decision task was used, priming for the same set of homophones was severely reduced. This latter finding suggests that phonological-articulatory processes do not play as large a role in mediating priming when target items are not named. Although the homophone priming was reduced in the lexical decision task, we did observe a small amount of priming when the target homophones were lower in frequency than the primes. This finding closely parallels a recent study by Grainger and Ferrand (1994), who also found minimal overall priming for homophones (5 ms) in the lexical decision task, but obtained more homophone priming when primes were higher in frequency than targets (24 ms) as opposed to when primes were lower in frequency (-14 ms; Experiment 1). It is also interesting to note that Grainger and Ferrand (1994) failed to obtain priming for low frequency homophone targets in the lexical decision task when pseudohomophones were included in the set of nonword foils (Experiment 2), providing a further restriction on the conditions in which homophone priming is obtained. Additional studies, however, have reported priming between pseudohomophones and word targets, such as mayd-MADE (Ferrand & Grainger, 1992, 1993, 1994), regardless of the target frequency. Thus, properties of the prime may also be relevant to whether or not phonological priming is obtained in the lexical decision task.5 However, even in these latter experiments, there was evidence that orthographic representations contributed to the priming results. The general conclusion that we draw from these and related findings is that the role of phonology in word priming depends on many factors, including the type of task Letter and word priming 29 (e.g., lexical decision vs. naming; e.g., Forster & Davis, 1991), the materials (e.g., high vs. low frequency words; e.g., Grainger & Ferrand, 1994), and the environment in which words are tested (e.g., the inclusion vs. exclusion of pseudohomophones in a lexical decision task; e.g., Davelaar, Coltheart, Besner, Jonasson, 1978). Note, the influence of these factors on priming presumably reflects their more general influence on word identification. These findings have clear implications for the DIS word priming results reported earlier. Given that the DIS words were predominately high in frequency, and given the large size of the DIS priming in the various tasks (much larger than any homophone priming effect we observed), we would argue that the robust DIS word priming was largely mediated by abstract orthographic codes. The implication of these results for the DIS letter priming results is less straightforward, but given that homophone and DIS letter priming is largely restricted to the naming tasks, it appears that phonological-articulatory processes support both forms of priming. However, to avoid the fallacy of generalizing from words to letters, it is important to directly assess the role of phonological processes in supporting DIS letter priming. This is the purpose of Experiment 6. Experiment 6 In this last experiment, we assessed homophone priming using letter targets. A difficulty in setting up such an experiment is that different letters never share the same name. Therefore, we assessed priming between homophone word-letter pairs, such as sea-C, as well as between repetitions (e.g., c-C). If homophone and repetition priming are equivalent, the findings would support the conclusion that letter priming is mediated entirely by phonological-articulatory processes. In addition, we assessed priming between Letter and word priming 30 phonologically similar letters (e.g., z-C) in an attempt to replicate the Arguin and Bub (1995) failure to obtain priming between such items. A combination of robust priming for homophones and little or no priming between phonologically similar letters would suggest that phonological-articulatory letter priming has a narrow generalization gradient, consistent with word priming results (Forster & Davis, 1991).6 Method Participants. Sixteen students from the University of Wisconsin-Madison completed the letter priming task in return for course credit. Materials and Design: Letters. The target letters consisted in C, Q, Y, and R that are homophonous with the word primes: sea, cue, why, and are, respectively. The letters z, k, i, and a served as phonologically similar primes to the targets, respectively. The experiment included Prime/Target Relation as a within-subject factor (repeated vs. homophonous vs. phonologically similar vs. non-repeated). Different baselines were used to assess homophone priming on the one hand, and phonologically similar and repetition priming on the other. The non-repeated condition for the homophones consisted in a random pairing of one of the word primes with the letter targets (e.g., sea-Q), whereas for the latter conditions it consisted in a random paring of one of the letter primes with the targets (e.g., z-Q). Each target letter was presented 5 times in each condition, leading to 20 trials per condition, for a total of 100 trials. The 100 trials were presented in a random order. Procedure. Each trial was organized in the following way. First a pattern mask (#*#) was presented for 500 ms which was replaced by the prime for 50 ms. The prime was immediately followed by the same pattern mask (#*#) for 33 ms, immediately Letter and word priming 31 followed by the target which was displayed for 500 ms. The second mask was included in order to mask the first and last letters of the word primes prior to the presentation of the targets, given that these word primes were longer than the letter targets. This particular pattern mask was included because it effectively masked the word primes. Participants completed a set of 18 practice trials prior to the experimental trials. The same set of items were included in the practice and experimental trials, and participants were not informed that the initial trials were practice. Participants were asked to name the target words as quickly as possible. The experimenter monitored participants responses, and discarded any trials in which a naming error or a dysfluency occurred Results The naming latencies in each condition are shown in Table 6. As can be seen in this table, robust repetition (21 ms) and homophone priming was obtained (20 ms), and little priming was obtained between phonologically similar letters (5 ms). An overall ANOVA carried out on the RT data revealed a significant effect of priming, [F1(4,60) = 7.98, MSe=229.19, p < .01; F2(4,76) = 8.59, MSe = 255.45, p < .01], and the small priming effect obtained between phonologically similar letters (5ms) did not achieve significance [F1(1,15) = 1.42, p > .1; F2(1,19) < 1]. An analysis carried out on the error results failed to reveal any significant effects [F1(4,60) = 1.42, p > .2; F2(4,76) = 1.52, p > .2]. One possible explanation for the homophone priming results that would not implicate phonological processes is that 2 of the prime words shared a letter with the target (why-Y and are-R). If priming was restricted to these items, it could be argued that Letter and word priming 32 the priming was mediated by the abstract letter codes common to these primes and targets. This possibility seems unlikely, given the significant priming results on the item analysis, but in order to directly check this possibility, we examined the priming effects obtained with the C&Q; and Y&R; targets separately. The Y&R; priming was similar in the homophone (26 ms) and repetition (24 ms) conditions, as was the case for the C&Q; priming in the homophone (13 ms) and repetition (18 ms) conditions. In addition, phonologically similar letter priming was reduced for both the Y&R; ( 8 ms) and C&Q; (1 ms) items. An analysis of this data was selectively carried out on the C&Q; condition, where the prime words and targets shared no letters. Once again, significant priming was found for RTs [F1(4,60) = 2.84, MSe = 419.10, p < .05,], and critically, a simple contrast revealed significant homophone priming, [F1(1,15) = 5.67, MSe = 254, p < .05]. Accordingly, the significant homophone priming must be attributed to phonologicalarticulatory processes. Discussion. Consistent with the word results of Experiment 5, robust homophone priming was obtained for letter targets in the naming task. Indeed, the homophone priming was equal in magnitude to the repetition priming, suggesting that all of the priming was mediated by phonological-articulatory codes. Thus, this last experiment provides direct evidence for our characterization of DIS letter priming, without making inferences from word data. In addition, our failure to obtain priming between phonologically similar letters indicates that phonological-articulatory priming is highly specific, and suggests that priming is only obtained when letters share the same name, or when words share the same name or onset (e.g., Forster & Davis, 1991).7 Letter and word priming 33 General Discussion Three main sets of results are reported in the present paper. First, robust priming was obtained for DIS and SIM letters in the naming task (Experiment 2), but priming was restricted to SIM letters in an alphabetic decision (Experiment 1), a vowel/consonant categorization (Experiment 3), and an identification (Experiment 4) task. Second, robust priming was obtained for DIS and SIM words in all tasks that we employed. These results are summarized in Table 7. Third, robust homophone priming was obtained between words (e.g., prime = sail, target = SALE) and between word-letter pairs (e.g., prime=sea, target = C) when the naming task was used in Experiments 5-6, but priming was greatly reduced between word homophones when the lexical decision task was used in Experiment 5. These results pose two obvious puzzles; namely, why is DIS letter priming, as opposed to DIS word priming, restricted to the naming task? And why is homophone priming reduced in the lexical decision compared to the naming task? We suggest that both puzzles can readily be explained by adopting the position that DIS word priming is largely mediated by abstract orthographic codes, whereas DIS letter priming is mediated by phonological-articulatory codes. On this account, DIS word priming should extend to all tasks, given that all tasks require the encoding of the orthographic properties of words, whereas DIS letter priming should largely be restricted to the naming task, given that this task selectively emphasizes the phonological and articulatory encoding of the target materials. The finding that both letter and word homophone priming is robust in the naming task supports this latter view. Letter and word priming 34 In the remainder of this section we attempt to characterize the representations that support letter and word priming in more detail. Abstract orthographic representations support word priming. In our view, the present results support the claim that abstract orthographic codes often mediate priming for words, and challenge the view of a number of authors who argue that priming is largely mediated by phonological representations (e.g., Lukatela and Turvey, 1994; Perfetti, Bell, & Delaney, 1988). Perhaps, the strongest advocates of this position are Lukatela and Turvey (1994) who claim that all word priming phenomena are phonological in nature, regardless of the task requirements. The authors base their hypothesis on the co-occurrence of cross-case and homophone priming in their naming experiments. That is, whenever they observed priming between upperand lower-case prime/targets (e.g., prime = SAIL, target = sail), they also observed priming between homophones (e.g., prime = SALE, target = sail), suggesting that the cross-case and homophone priming is mediated by the same representations; namely, phonological codes. They then extended this analysis, and questioned the existence of abstract orthographic codes altogether. They wrote: "To the extent that the hypothesis of abstract graphemes is coupled with the absence of evidence for a true phonological priming effect .... the present results suggest that this hypothesis is unwarranted. A better hypothesis, it seems, is that variants of a letter in English are functionally equivalent because they map to an invariant configuration of phonological features" (p. 347). However, the combination of robust cross-case priming and null homophone priming for high frequency targets in the lexical decision task (Experiment 5) contradicts their claim that cross-case and homophone priming always co-occur. Accordingly, our Letter and word priming 35 results support the existence of abstract orthographic codes, and furthermore, indicate that these codes support word priming. It is also worth mentioning that the existence of abstract orthographic word codes is supported by a number of different studies reported in the literature, including studies on the word superiority effect (Bowers, Bub & Arguin, 1996; McClelland, 1976); long term priming phenomena (Bowers, 1996, Bowers & Michita, in press; Marsolek, Kosslyn, & Squire, 1992); eye-tracking experiments (Rayner, McConkie, & Zola, 1980); and matching experiments (Besner, Coltheart, & Davelaar, 1984), among others. We suggest that Lukatela and Turvey's reliance on the naming task has led them to overemphasize the role of phonological codes in word processing. Although orthographic codes play a critical role in DIS word priming in the lexical decision task, the robust homophone priming observed in the naming task (Experiments 5-6, above; Lukatela & Turvey, 1994) indicate that phonologicalarticulatory representations play a critical role when participants name the targets. Although the present experiments do not allow us to disentangle the relative contribution of phonological vs. articulatory priming when the naming task is employed, a number of findings suggest that the articulatory contribution is relatively small. The most relevant finding was reported by Lukatela and Turvey (1994), who assessed homophone priming for the same set of words that we included in our study. In their experiments, they included a condition in which targets were preceded by words that shared the same initial phonemes (e.g., TOLD/toad), allowing a direct comparison between onset and homophone priming (they used the term quasi homographic to describe the condition in which words shared initial phonemes). They found a significant 12 ms onset effect and a 29 ms homophone priming effect when targets were low in frequency compared to primes, Letter and word priming 36 and a nonsignificant 6 ms onset and 27 ms homophone effect when targets were high in frequency in comparison to primes. Thus, these findings suggest that only a small proportion of the priming we obtained in the naming task should be attributed to articulatory effects. In addition to the phonological-articulatory and abstract orthographic contributions to word priming, visually specific perceptual codes can also contribute to priming given that more priming was obtained for SIM compared to DIS words in the perceptual identification task. It is important to emphasize, however, that the SIM and DIS word priming was equivalent in the lexical decision and noun/verb tasks, and in the identification task, the advantage of SIM over DIS words was small (9%) relative to the overall amount of SIM priming (43%). Thus, the contribution of the visually specific codes to word priming is relatively modest. The nature of these specific codes is taken up in some more detail when the letter priming results are discussed. One final issue regarding the representations that underlie the word priming results should be discussed here. We have been arguing that abstract orthographic representations are largely responsible for the obtained priming effects (at least in nonnaming tasks), but we have not discussed the nature of these orthographic codes. One possibility is that sub-lexical orthographic representations mediate priming -such as abstract, bigram or trigram codes -consistent with the hypothesis advanced by Humphreys and colleagues (Humphreys et al., 1988, 1990). In support of this view are the various reports that priming extends to pseudowords in the naming and perceptual identification tasks (e.g., Humphreys et al., 1990; Masson, 1991), and this approach is congruent with theories that deny the existence of lexical-orthographic knowledge (e.g., Letter and word priming 37 Seidenberg & McClellend, 1989). Another possibility, however, is that lexical-orthographic representations mediate priming, consistent with the view of Forster and his colleagues. In support of this latter view, priming is often restricted to words when the lexical decision task is employed (e.g., Forster & Davis, 1984), and furthermore, this approach is congruent with the view that reading involves accessing lexicalorthographic codes (e.g., Forster et al., 1984, 1987, 1991; McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981). Of course, a third possibility is that word priming effects reflect a combination of both lexical and sub-lexical factors. A recent study by Bowers, Arguin & Bub (1996) suggests that sub-lexical codes can in fact support priming. The study assessed masked priming for words and pseudowords in a patient (IH) who reads in a letter-by-letter fashion; that is, IH read very slowly, and his reading times are a linear function of word length, with approximately 500 ms increase in reading reaction times with each additional letter. Despite his slow reading, IH showed robust priming in the naming task for both words and pseudowords when primes were displayed for 100 ms. Furthermore, he failed to show pseudoword priming when primes and targets only differed in the fourth letter position, for instance, tern/TERB. This latter finding indicates that the pseudoword priming was not the product of an onset effect. Thus, the robust repetition priming for pseudowords must have been mediated by the activation of sub-lexical codes -either orthographic or phonological. Given that IH has slow access to phonological knowledge, as revealed by his failure to obtain masked homophone priming in the naming task (Arguin, Bub, & Bowers, submitted), it seems likely that sub-lexical orthographic codes were responsible, consistent Letter and word priming 38 with Humphreys et al. (1990). But clearly, these findings are consistent with the view that lexical representations also contribute to masked priming phenomena. Finally, we would like to conclude this section by briefly noting some of the more general implications of the homophone priming results reported in Experiment 5. A number of authors have argued that access to phonology is virtually always prior, and even necessary for visual word recognition during reading (e.g., Lukatela and Turvey, 1994; Perfetti, Bell, & Delaney, 1988; Van Orden, 1987). The key evidence presented in support of this conclusion comes from studies that reported a high proportion of homophone errors in semantic categorization tasks (e.g., participants mistakenly categorize rows as a flower; Van Orden, 1987), and studies that reported masked priming between homophones (e.g., Perfetti et al., 1988; Perfetti & Bell, 1991). However, in contrast to this position, Jared, McRae, & Seidenberg (1990) reported that homophone errors in the semantic categorization task were restricted to low frequency homophone targets, suggesting that although phonology can play an important role in word identification, its role is more pronounced for low frequency words (See also Coltheart, Patterson, & Leahy, (1994) for further restrictions regarding the conditions in which homophone errors are made). Our findings provide similar constraints regarding the conditions in which phonological codes are used in word processing tasks, given that homophone priming in the lexical decision task was restricted to the low-frequency targets. Thus, we would suggest that the strong conclusion that phonology is virtually always necessary for visual word identification is unwarranted (also see Baluch & Besner, 1991; Besner, Twilley, McCann, & Seergobin, 1990; Tabossi & Laghi, 1992; Waters & Seidenberg, 1985; among others). Note, it may well be the case that phonological codes Letter and word priming 39 are computed automatically following the visual presentation of a word, but they may not always be used for identification (for a discussion for the distinction between whether phonology is computed vs. used, see Besner, Dennis, & Davelaar, 1985). Phonological codes and articulatory processes mediate letter priming In sharp contrast to word priming, we believe DIS letter priming is exclusively mediated by phonological-articulatory processes. This conclusion is based on (a) the equivalent homophone and cross-case letter priming we observed (Experiment 6), and (b) the finding that DIS letter priming was obtained in the naming task (Experiment 2), but not in tasks that do not require participants to phonologically encode and articulate letters (Experiments 1, 3, & 4). In addition, converging evidence for this position can be found in a number of related studies. For example, Bowers et al. (1996) reported that the letterby-letter patient (IH) failed to show priming for DIS letters when primes were displayed for 100 ms in the naming task, even though this same prime duration supported word priming in IH. A plausible interpretation of this finding is that IH was able to gain fast access to orthographic word knowledge, but that his letter-by-letter reading prevented him from gaining fast access to phonological-articulatory processes, which in turn selectively impaired his priming for letters. As in the case of words, it will be important to determine the relative role that phonological and articulatory effects play in DIS letter priming. That is, at least some of the observed priming may reflect the fact that participants prepared to articulate the prime letter prior to the encoding of the target, which facilitated target naming when prime and target shared the same name, and inhibited target naming otherwise. However, the relative role of these factors is impossible to determine at present. Letter and word priming 40 Although DIS letter priming was restricted to the naming task, there was a consistent trend for SIM letter priming in all tasks, with substantial priming in the identification task. Thus it appears that perceptually specific representations can also support letter priming, as was the case with words in the identification task. At present, however, we can only speculate about the nature of these representations. Presumably, these codes are not low-level (iconic) visual traces of the prime since lowerand uppercase SIM letters differ in size among other attributes. Instead, these codes must map together items that share critical (unspecified) visual attributes. One possibility is that these codes are visually specific letter tokens that map onto abstract letter representations, consistent with the view of Arguin and Bub (1995). So on this view, SIM priming is mediated by representations within the orthographic system itself. Another possibility is that these representations lie outside the orthographic system altogether. For example, the SIM priming may be mediated by representations in the right hemisphere that code visual information in a more specific format, whereas DIS priming is mediated by abstract representations within the orthographic system in the left hemisphere (Marsolek, Kosslyn, & Squire, 1992; Marsolek, 1995). Whatever the case, the results clearly indicate the importance of considering the perceptual attributes of letters and words when employing priming paradigms. General implications regarding the orthographic system. Our failure to obtain DIS letter priming in non-naming tasks may have implications regarding the representational status of letters; that is, the results might be taken to indicate that abstract letter codes do not exist. On this view, orthographic letter codes are case specific, and these codes would presumably connect to abstract word or sub-lexical representations, providing a basis for Letter and word priming 41 abstract orthographic word priming in the absence of letter priming. Although we cannot unequivocally rule out this hypothesis, we are reluctant to advocate this position given that some findings directly support the existence of abstract letter codes (not inferred from word or pseudoword results). For example, some patients with developmental or acquired reading disorders have difficulties naming individual letters, but nevertheless, they have no problems matching upperand lower-case letters that are visually dissimilar (e.g., Rynard & Besner, 1987; Coltheart, 1981). It is plausible to assume that in order to correctly perform the matching task, these individuals access abstract orthographic letter codes (for additional evidence in direct support of abstract letter codes, see Besner & Jolicoeur, submitted; Mozer, 1989; for another interpretation of cross-case letter matching, see Boles & Eveland, 1983). It must be acknowledged, however, that the majority of research taken to support abstract letter codes has been obtained using word and pseudoword materials, and accordingly, additional research on this issue is needed. Alternatively, the absence of DIS priming in non-naming tasks may reflect the specific processes involved in reading single-letters, in which case, the results do not speak to representational issues. On this, our preferred view, the absence of DIS letter priming reflects the short lasting activation of abstract letter codes following the presentation of the prime, due either to a fast decay rate of letter activation, or to an active suppression of this activation when new letter information (the target) arrives. In both cases, the prior encoding of the prime would not facilitate the processing of the target since the letter activation would be at baseline levels again when the target is encoded. In order to account for the overall pattern of letter and word priming results, we would have to assume that the activation of lexical orthographic knowledge is moreorthographic word priming in the absence of letter priming. Although we cannot unequivocally rule out this hypothesis, we are reluctant to advocate this position given that some findings directly support the existence of abstract letter codes (not inferred from word or pseudoword results). For example, some patients with developmental or acquired reading disorders have difficulties naming individual letters, but nevertheless, they have no problems matching upperand lower-case letters that are visually dissimilar (e.g., Rynard & Besner, 1987; Coltheart, 1981). It is plausible to assume that in order to correctly perform the matching task, these individuals access abstract orthographic letter codes (for additional evidence in direct support of abstract letter codes, see Besner & Jolicoeur, submitted; Mozer, 1989; for another interpretation of cross-case letter matching, see Boles & Eveland, 1983). It must be acknowledged, however, that the majority of research taken to support abstract letter codes has been obtained using word and pseudoword materials, and accordingly, additional research on this issue is needed. Alternatively, the absence of DIS priming in non-naming tasks may reflect the specific processes involved in reading single-letters, in which case, the results do not speak to representational issues. On this, our preferred view, the absence of DIS letter priming reflects the short lasting activation of abstract letter codes following the presentation of the prime, due either to a fast decay rate of letter activation, or to an active suppression of this activation when new letter information (the target) arrives. In both cases, the prior encoding of the prime would not facilitate the processing of the target since the letter activation would be at baseline levels again when the target is encoded. In order to account for the overall pattern of letter and word priming results, we would have to assume that the activation of lexical orthographic knowledge is more Letter and word priming 42 persistent than the activation of single letter codes, as well as assume that the decay rate of phonological codes is relatively slow (or the activation of these codes is not suppressed by new information). In this scenario, word priming would indeed extend to all tasks and would reflect the activation of abstract orthographic codes, and phonological codes would support DIS letter and homophone (and DIS word) priming whenever the naming task was employed. Of course, we readily admit that it is speculative to suggest that the activation of orthographic letter codes is less persistent than orthographic word codes or phonological letter codes, and additional work is required before any firm conclusions can be drawn. Still, the present results highlight the possibility that the activation time course for abstract orthographic letter and word representations, as well as phonological letter codes, are different. Computational accounts of letter and word processing have typically assumed, for the sake of parsimony, that the activation properties of orthographic letter and word codes are the same (McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981; Jacobs & Grainger, 1992). The present findings should at least raise concerns about this assumption. In summary, we have provided strong evidence that orthographic representations support priming for words, while phonological-articulatory codes support priming for single-letters. We suggest that both abstract letter and word codes exist in the orthographic system, but for some reason, only orthographic word codes support priming. Letter and word priming 43 ReferencesAdams, M.J. (1979). Models of word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 11,133-176.Allen, P.A., Wallace, B. & Weber, T.A. (1995). Influence of case type, wordfrequency, and exposure duration on visual word recognition. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 914-934.Arguin, M. & Bub, D. (1995). Priming and response s/election processes in letterclassification and identification tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: HumanPerception and Performance, 21, 1199-1219.Arguin, M, Bub, D., & Bowers, J.S. (submitted). Extent and limits of covertlexical activation in letter-by-letter reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology.Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (1991). Visual word recognition: Evidence for strategiccontrol of lexical and nonlexical routines in oral reading. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 644-652.Besner, D., Coltheart, M. & Davelaar, E. (1984). Basic Processes in reading:Computation of abstract letter identities. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 38, 126-134.Besner, D., Dennis, I., & Davelaar, E. (1985). Reading without phonology?Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 477-492.Besner, D. & Johnston, J.C. (1989). Reading and the mental lexicon.: On theuptake of visual information. In W. Marslen-Wilson (Ed.), Lexical representations andprocesses (pp. 291-316). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Besner, D. & Jolicoeur, P. (submitted). Basic processes in reading: Cross-caseletter matching without phonology. Letter and word priming 44 Besner, D., Twilley, L., McCann, R.S., & Seergobin, K (1990). On theassociation between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? PsychologicalReview, 97, 432-446.Bowers, J.S. (1996). Different perceptual codes support word and pseudowordpriming: Was Morton right all along? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1336-1353.Bowers, J.S., Arguin, M. & Bub, D. (1996). Fast and specific access toorthographic knowledge in a case of letter-by-letter surface alexia. CognitiveNeuropsychology, 13, 525-567.Bowers, J.S., Bub, D. & Arguin, M. (1996). A characterization of the wordsuperiority effect in a case of letter-by-letter surface alexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology,13, 415-441.Bowers, J .S., & Michita, Y. (in press). An investigaton into the structure andacquisition of orthographic knowledge: Evidence from cross-script Kanji-Hiraganapriming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.Boles, D.B., & Clifford, J.E. (1989). An upperand lower case alphabeticsimilarity matrix, with derived generation similarity values. Behavior Research Methods,Instruments, & Computers, 12, 597-586.Boles, D.B., & Eveland, D.C. (1983). Visual and phonetic codes and the processof generation in letter matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perceptionand Performance, 9, 657-674.Coltheart, M. (1981). Disorders of reading and their implications for models ofnormal reading. Visible Language, 3, 245-286 Letter and word priming 45 Coltheart, M. & Rastle, K. (1994). Serial Processing in reading aloud: Evidencefor dual route models of reading, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perceptionand Performance 20, 1197-1211.Coltheart, V., Patterson K. & Leahy, J. (1994) When a ROWS is a ROSE: Phonologicaleffects in written word comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.Davelaar, E., Coltheart 1, M. & Besner, D., & Jonasson, J.T. (1978). Phonologicalrecoding and lexical access. Memory & Cognition, 6, 391-402.Davis, C. (1990). Masked priming effects in speeded classification tasks.Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology, Monash University.Davis, C. & Forster, K.I. (1994). Masked orthographic priming: The effect ofprime-target legibility. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 673-697.Evett, L.J. & Humphreys, G.W. (1981). The use of abstract graphemicinformation in lexical access. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 325-350.Ferrand, L. & Grainger, J. (1992). Phonology and orthography in visual wordrecognition: Evidence from masked nonword priming. Quarterly Journal of ExperimentalPsychology, 45A, 353-372.Ferrand, L. & Grainger, J. (1993). The time-course of orthographic andphonological code activation in the early phases of visual word recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31, 119-122.Ferrand, L. & Grainger, J. (1994). Effects of orthography are independent of phonology in masked form priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Letter and word priming 46 Forster, K.I. (1992). Lexical effects in masked form-priming. Paper presented atthe 33'rd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis, Missouri.Forster, K.I. (1993). Form-priming and temporal integration in word recognition.In G.T.M. Altmann & R.C. Shillcock (Eds.). Cognitive Models of Speech Processing.The Sperlonga meeting, II. Hove, Erlbaum.Forster, K.I. & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation inlexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,10,680-698.Forster, K.I., & Davis, C. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in thenaming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory andLanguage, 30, 1-25.Francis, W.N., & Kucera, H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage:Lexicon and grammar. Houghton Mifflin Company, Boston.Grainger, J. & Ferrand, L. (1994). Phonology and orthography in visual wordrecognition: Effects of masked homophone primes. Journal of Memory and Language,33, 218-233.Henderson, L., Wallis, J., & Knight, D. (1984). Morphemic structure and lexicalaccess. In H. Bouma & D. Bouhuis (Eds.)., Attention & Performance X. London:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.Humphreys, G.W., Besner, D. & Quinlan P.T. (1988). Event perception and theword repetition effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 51-67.Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J., & Taylor, D.E. (1982). Automatic phonologicalpriming in visual word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 10, 576-590 Letter and word priming 47 Humphreys, G.W., Evett, L.J., & Quinlan, P.T. (1990). Orthographic processingin visual word identification. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 517-560.Jacobs, A.M., & Grainger, J. (1991). Automatic letter priming in an alphabeticdecision task. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 43-52.Jacobs, A.M., & Grainger, J. (1992). Testing a semi-stochastic variant of theinteractive activation model in different word recognition experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1174-1188.Jacobs, A.M. Grainger, & J. Ferrand, L. (1995) The incremental primingtechnique: A method for determining within-condition priming effects. Perception andPsychophysics, 57, 1101-1110.Lukatela, G., & Turvey, M.T. (1994). Visual lexical access is initiallyphonological: 2. Evidence from phonological priming by homophones andpseudohomophones. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 331-353.Marsolek, C.J. (1995). Abstract visual-form representations in the left cerebralhemisphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,21, 375-386.Marsolek, C.J., Kosslyn, S.M. & Squire, L.R. (1992). Form-specific visualpriming in the right cerebral hemisphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,Memory, and Cognition, 18, 492-508.Masson, M.E.J. (1991). Word repetition as the eyes see it. Paper presented atthe32'nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, San Francisco. Letter and word priming 48 McClelland, J.L. (1976). Preliminary letter identification in the perception ofwords and nonwords. Journal of Experimental psychology: Human Perception andPerformance, 3, 80-91.McClelland, J.L. & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model ofcontext effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. PsychologicalReview, 88, 375-407.Meyer, D.E., & Schvaneveldt, R.W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs ofwords: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology, 91, 227-234.Mozer, M. (1989). Types and tokens in visual letter perception. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 15, 287-303..Perfetti, C.A., Bell, L.C., & Delaney, S. (1988). Automatic (prelexical) phoneticactivation in silent word reading: Evidence from backward masking. Journal of Memoryand Language, 27, 59-70.Perfetti, C.A., & Bell, L. (1991). Phonemic activation during the first 40 ms ofword identification: Evidence from backward masking and priming. Journal of Memoryand Language, 30, 473-485.Rayner, K., McConkie, G.W., & Zola, D. (1980). Integrating information acrosseye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 206-226.Reicher, G.M. (1969). Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulusmaterial. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 274-280. Letter and word priming 49 Rynard, D. & Besner, D. (1987). Basic processes in reading: On the developmentof cross-case letter matching without reference to phonology. Bulletin of thePsychonomic Society, 25, 361-368.Seidenberg, M.S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmentalmodel of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523-568.Segui, J. & Grainger, J. (1990). Priming word recognition with orthographicneighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 65-76.Stone, G.O., & Van Orden G.C. (1993). Strategic control of processing in wordrecognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,19, 744-.Tabossi, P., & Laghi, L. (1992). Semantic priming in the pronunciation of wordsin two writing systems: Italian and English. Memory & Cognition, 20, 315-328.Verstaen, A. Humphreys, G.W. Olson, A. & D'Ydewalle, G. (1996) Are phonemiceffects in backward masking evidence for automatic prelexical phonemic activation invisual word recognition? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 335-356.Waters, G.S., & Seidenberg, M. (1985). Spelling-sound effects in reading: Timecourse and decision criteria. Memory & Cognition, 13, 557-572. Letter and word priming 50 Footnotes1. It is important to emphasize that different processes may mediate masked compared to otherforms of priming, such as semantic (e.g., Meyer & Schvaneveldt, 1971) or long-term (e.g.,Bowers, 1996) priming. Indeed, there is good evidence that different mechanisms support thesevarious phenomena (e.g., Forster & Davis, 1984; Henderson, Wallis, & Knight, 1984; Stolz &Besner, submitted). In the following, the term “priming” is used descriptively to denote anyfacilitation in processing a target when it is preceded by a related compared to an unrelated prime.2. Jacobs & Grainger did in fact assess priming for DIS letters in their Experiment 3, but aninappropriate baseline was employed in this study (cf., Arguin & Bub, 1995). In the criticalexperiment that included the proper baseline (Experiment 1), SIM and DIS letters were intermixed.3. Given that there is no time pressure to respond in the identification task, we reasoned guessingmight be a problem if a small set of words from the large set of possible words was repeatedmultiple times. Note, this is not as much of a problem for the letter experiment, where more thanhalf of the possible items were repeatedly presented, rendering guessing from past trials lessinformative. Nor is this a problem in the other word priming tasks since participants wererequired to responded quickly.4. When prime/target words and letters were presented 34 ms in a pilot experiment, participantsidentified many more words compared to letters in the baseline conditions, presumably because ofthe word superiority effect (Reicher, 1969).5. Interesting, Hino, Lupker, Ogawa, & Sears (unpublished) observed equally small (non-significant) homophone masked priming between Kanji words in Japanese (15 ms) and betweenpseudoword Kanji primes and Kanji word targets (11 ms) when the lexical decision task was Letter and word priming 51 employed, contrary to Ferrand et al.. The authors also found highly significant homophonepriming when these same items were included in a naming task, consistent with the present finding.6. We would like to thank Martin Arguin and Derek Besner for helpful discussion which lead tothe development of this experiment7. It might be argued that our failure to obtain priming between phonologically similar letters wasdue to the fact that we included some prime/target letters there were only moderately similarphonologically, i.e., the pairs k-Q and a-R. These pairs were included because we were restrictedto the target letters Q and R given that they are homophonous with words. It should be noted thatwe have also run a priming experiment when the letters p/B, t/D, c/Z, and j/G were included asprimes and targets, and again, we found little evidence of priming (7 ms of priming, with slightlymore errors in the repeated condition). And as noted above, Arguin and Bub (1995) also reportedthe same null finding with a different set of letters. Letter and word priming 52

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Orthographic, phonological, and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming.

A series of experiments assessed masked priming for letters and words that are visually similar (SIM) and dissimilar (DIS) in upper- and lowercase formats. For letters, robust DIS priming was obtained in a naming task, but this priming did not extend to a variety of non-naming tasks. For words, robust DIS priming was obtained in both naming and non-naming tasks. SIM letter and word priming exte...

متن کامل

Visual and phonological codes in letter and word recognition: evidence from incremental priming.

Critical issues in letter and word priming were investigated using the novel incremental priming technique. This technique adds a parametric manipulation of prime duration (or prime intensity) to the traditional design of a fast masked priming study. By doing so, additional information on the time course and nature of priming effects can be obtained. In Experiment 1, cross-case letter priming (...

متن کامل

Fast and specific access to orthographic knowledge in a case of letter - by - letter surface alexia

A series of experiments assessed priming for single letters and words in a letter-by-letter reader (IH) when primes were displayed briefly (between 100-500 ms) and masked. Consistent with previous claims that letter-by-letter readers have difficulties accessing orthographic letter codes, IH failed to show normal cross-case priming for single letters in a naming task (e.g., a/A). Nevertheless, I...

متن کامل

Experimental Psychology, in press Does kaniso activate CASINO? Input coding schemes and phonology in visual-word recognition

Most recent input coding schemes in visual-word recognition assume that letter position coding is orthographic rather than phonological in nature (e.g., SOLAR, open-bigram, SERIOL, and overlap). This assumption has been drawn –in part– by the fact that the transposed-letter effect (e.g., caniso activates CASINO) seems to be (mostly) insensitive to phonological manipulations (e.g., Perea & Carre...

متن کامل

Does Kaniso activate CASINO?: input coding schemes and phonology in visual-word recognition.

Most recent input coding schemes in visual-word recognition assume that letter position coding is orthographic rather than phonological in nature (e.g., SOLAR, open-bigram, SERIOL, and overlap). This assumption has been drawn - in part - by the fact that the transposed-letter effect (e.g., caniso activates CASINO) seems to be (mostly) insensitive to phonological manipulations (e.g., Perea & Car...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2000